Questions and answers about
the economy.

How can we reduce gender gaps in mathematics education?

Underperformance by girls and young women in mathematics relative to their male peers is a loss of talent that may hinder productivity growth. New developments in our understanding of how gender stereotypes originate and evolve can help us to reduce gender differences in achievement.

Boys tend to outperform girls in mathematics. This is particularly the case among students who achieve the highest grades (Hyde and Mertz, 2009; Ellison and Swanson, 2010).

This is significant as gender differences in maths performance can explain an important part of the gender gap in educational choices and future labour market outcomes. Despite progress in women’s college preparation and graduation rates over the last 70 years, women remain under-represented in the higher-paying and maths-intensive STEM fields (science, technology, engineering and maths). They also tend to choose degrees with about 6% lower average earnings than men and 10% lower among the highest earners, those above the 90th percentile of earnings (Bertrand, 2018).

How can we address gender gaps in maths education? This article looks at how new developments in gender economics are changing our understanding of the maths gender gap and reshaping policy as a result.

How differently do girls and boys perform in mathematics?

At the age of 11 in the UK, boys are four percentage points more likely to achieve the expected standard in maths than girls. They are also eight percentage points more likely to achieve the higher standard in standardised tests (Cavaglia et al, 2020).

A similar pattern is found in other countries. On average, girls’ maths results are lower than boys’ at the age of 15 (see, for example, Borgonovi et al, 2018, which analyses data from 13 OECD countries). These differences are seen particularly among the most able students (Machin and Pekkarinen, 2008; Ellison and Swanson, 2010).

Recent evidence from several countries shows that while the gender gap in maths is almost negligible in primary school, it more than triples between the ages of 9-10 and 15-16 (Bharadwaj et al, 2016; Contini et al, 2017; Ellison and Swanson, 2018; Borra et al, 2021; Borgonovi et al, 2018). This gap in maths performance equates to girls missing around four months of schooling (Woessmann, 2016).

Girls’ underperformance in maths represents a potential loss of talent, which may lead to lower productivity and economic growth (Hsieh et al, 2019).

There is no biological reason why boys should be better at maths than girls. In fact, if one were to choose two people at random, the differences across a multitude of traits and abilities between a man and a woman (or a boy and a girl) will be much smaller than the differences between two women/girls or between two men/boys (Hyde, 2005; Bertrand, 2020).

Even for the small number of traits for which sex differences are significant (such as spatial thinking, sensation-seeking, physical aggression and some sexual behaviours), it is increasingly becoming more difficult to justify these differences as biologically driven, given that the newest findings in neuroscience suggest that the brain changes and adapts according to the social environment (Rippon, 2019).

What role do gender stereotypes play?

Gender stereotypes are fixed ideas about what someone of a particular gender is like (descriptive) or should be like (prescriptive), simply for the mere fact of belonging to a certain gender group. Common gender stereotypes are that girls are biologically worse than boys at maths, that girls are more risk-averse than boys and that mothers should stop work to look after young children.

Stereotypes influence women’s and girls’ preferences and expectations. Prescriptive stereotypes such as conservative beliefs about the role of women in society, rather than biology, have long been named as a factor that can explain much of the gender gap in mathematical attainment between boys and girls across countries (Guiso et al, 2008; Nollenberger et al, 2016).

Social expectations lower women’s incentives to do well. The lower performance of girls versus boys in maths may also be the result of girls internalising a socially constructed behaviour in line with widely held descriptive gender stereotypes (for example, that girls shy away from competition and take lower risks) in contexts in which the task at hand has a strong gender stereotype associated to it, such as in mathematical assessments (Niederle, 2017; Iriberri and Rey-Biel, 2019).

Stereotypes also constrain women’s and girls’ choices, even when they do not internalise them.One study finds that teachers with more implicit gender stereotypical beliefs about girls’ ability to do maths (as measured by the Gender-Science Implicit Association Test) advise girls to pursue less maths-intensive subjects (Carlana, 2019). It also finds that girls taught by these teachers display lower confidence in the subject.

The influence of social psychology has led to a growing acceptance among economists that stereotyping behaviour may also arise from a fast and unconscious (or ‘implicit’) thought process as much as from a slow and conscious process (Bertrand et al, 2005; Kahneman, 2011).

Implicit stereotyping behaviour occurs even under scenarios of ‘perfect information’. Whereas economists have traditionally assumed that stereotypes kick in when we know little about a person, new evidence is beginning to emerge about stereotyping occurring even in family and classroom settings where parents and teachers know a lot about their children and students (Alesina et al, 2018; Dossi et al, 2021a,  2021b).

Either because girls receive less resources from parents and teachers when they pursue male-dominated subjects such as maths, or because girls internalise these gender-stereotypical preferences, the end result is that girls end up choosing subjects of study that are ‘believed appropriate’ for their gender, and/or do worse in male-dominated subjects such as maths.

How is policy being reshaped?

When unconscious gender stereotyping is at play, ‘coercive policies’ that explicitly tell people what to do may not be as effective as interventions involving small nudges (Bohnet et al, 2016). A major objective of behavioural policy interventions is to avoid unconscious stereotypes entering the decision-making process in the first place.

One type of intervention consists of tackling implicit stereotyping on the part of teachers (and parents) by using awareness techniques so that more conscious (unbiased) processes kick in. A popular tool for addressing unconscious stereotyping is revealing biases. This can be done, for example, through an implicit association test (IAT), a computer-based tool developed by social psychologists, which is designed to minimise the risk of social desirability bias (Greenwald et al, 2009).

An increasing number of firms and institutions, including Harvard University, administer the IAT to their employees. In an educational context, one study shows that revealing stereotypes to teachers via the IAT could be a powerful intervention to decrease gender discrimination in grading (Alesina et al, 2018). Other awareness interventions targeting decision-makers are ‘unconscious bias training’ and ‘perspective-taking training’.

An alternative to awareness interventions is the implementation of measures that change the context in which decisions are made, so that a more conscious process comes into effect. Researchers suggest that a large part of the gender gap in maths at high levels can be explained by the different ways in which men and women respond to competitive test-taking environments (Niederle and Vesterlund, 2010)

The rise in girls’ underperformance in maths has been attributed to a higher aversion to competitive pressure (Iriberri and Rey-Biel, 2019). Others show that girls outperform boys in all tests but to a greater extent when the stakes are low (Azmat et al, 2016). These results can help policy-makers to design tests that reduce gender differences in performance resulting from factors unrelated to true ability.

Policy interventions aimed at girls start early in childhood. Certain policies are aimed at preventing girls (and boys) from implicitly internalising gender stereotypes by not exposing them to stereotypical roles. These include having female role models, such as women maths teachers, adopting gender-neutral language and using textbooks and other teaching materials that challenge the prevalence of gender stereotypes.

This is important as it has been found that three-quarters of the people mentioned in economics textbooks, whether real or imagined, are men. The under-representation of women in economics book can reinforce the notion that economics is not a discipline for women (Stevenson and Zlotnick, 2018).

Recent work tested whether a one-hour exposure to external female role models with a background in science affects students’ perceptions and choice of field of study. It finds that the intervention increased the share of girls in the final year of school enrolling in selective (male-dominated) STEM programmes in higher education from 11% to 14.5% (Breda et al, 2020) .

Researchers have also found that young women assigned to all-female classes in their first year of university are roughly 57% less likely to drop out and 61% more likely to get a top ranked degree under the UK system (Booth et al, 2018).

Other interventions include making girls more prepared to succeed, for example, by promoting resilience and grit, skills that are highly predictive of achievement. For example, when children are exposed to a world view that emphasises the role of effort in achievement and encourages perseverance, the gender gap in the willingness to compete disappears (Alan and Ertac, 2019). This shows that elimination of this gap implies significant efficiency gains.

Prescriptive gender stereotypes are more often than not based on descriptive biased beliefs about girls’ skills and abilities and these contribute to the maths gender gap. This matters economically because talent is lost and productivity suffers. As a result, policy-makers need to tackle unconscious and conscious biases.

Whereas awareness interventions have become popular because they can be cost-effective and easy to implement, sometimes challenging the stereotyping habits of decision-makers such as teachers and parents can backfire. For example, when provided with feedback about their own implicit associations, people may react defensively and question the validity of the bias test (O’Brien et al, 2010; Sukhera and Watling, 2018).

Similarly, unconscious bias training programmes can be very context-specific and their external validity is often questioned (Bertrand, 2020). Further, the underlying assumption of interventions that push girls to be more like boys is that the returns to these skills are the same across genders. Yet there is evidence that girls may not benefit in the same way and could even be penalised when acting like boys (Exeley et al, 2018).

When gender-entrenched stereotypes are not unconscious – but rather conscious and explicit within the education system – avoiding gender stereotypes in decision-making requires changing biased beliefs. Recent psychological research shows that stereotypical beliefs about girls’ (and women’s) skills and abilities have indeed changed over time.

Economists are starting to recognise the malleable nature of stereotypes (Lundberg, 2022). Understanding what promotes change is a fruitful avenue for future economics research and can help us to design interventions to close the maths performance gap between girls and boys.

Where can I find out more?

Who are experts on this question?

  • John Jerrim, University College London
  • Sandra McNally, University of Surrey
  • Cheti Nicoletti, University of York
  • Valentina Tonei, University of Southampton
  • Marina Della Giusta, University of Reading
  • Sule Alan, European Institute Florence
  • Michela Carlana, Harvard University
  • Nagore Iriberry, Universidad del Pais Vasco
  • Ghazala Azmat, Sciences Po
  • Shelly Lundberg, University of Santa Barbara
Authors: Almudena Sevilla (UCL) and Pilar Cuevas-Ruiz (UCL)
Photo by DragonImages on iStock
Recent Questions
View all articles
Do you have a question surrounding any of these topics? Or are you an economist and have an answer?
Ask a Question
OR
Submit Evidence